Pragmatic Judgment

argument talkers yell 2

The result of …inappropriate pragmatic judgment

The skill of pragmatic judgment, as it is commonly known, involves forming appropriate social language responses. In other words it’s saying the right thing at the right time. This is not always easy to measure or even verify. Pragmatic judgment also involves prior knowledge, and knowledge of a conversational partner’s prior knowledge. Sometimes social language involves initiation (e.g. “Hello), while other times the reaction is a response (e.g. “You’re Welcome). At times it is appropriate to not respond in an expected manner. This occurs when conversational maxims are deliberately flouted for reasons such as sarcasm, intentional overstatement or understatement (e.g. Grice, 1975).

Frequency and effectiveness of social response has been shown to significantly affect aspects of life as diverse as interpersonal relationship and occupation (e.g. Swann and Rentfrow, 2001). Pragmatic competency is assessed through such activities as requiring recognition of appropriate topics for conversation; selection of relevant information for directions or requests; initiation of conversation or turn-taking; adjusting communication to situational factors such as age or relationship; using language for expression of gratitude, sorrow, and other feelings; and judgment of the pragmatic appropriateness of the language behavior of others who are engaged in these activities (Carrow-Woolfolk, 1999).

Commonly used assessments with pragmatic judgment include the TOPL and the CASL tests. Informal assessment in natural settings may provide more reliable information regarding pragmatic judgment than formal assessment. Assisting teachers complete pragmatic checklists, such as that provided with the CELF-5, provides information regarding skills specific to the classroom.  Read on for a very shortened hierarchy of possible pragmatic judgment goals.

Continue reading →

Verb Tense

Chances are that if you have a kid with a language disorder, you have a kid with verb tense problems.  Verb tense overlaps with many language skills, such as subject-verb agreement, production of infinitive verbs, irregular past tense, question formation, and helping verbs. Research suggests that omission of tense marker (“zero marking”) is the most prevalent kind of tense error in children with SLI (Marchman, Wulfeck, Weimer, 1999). Tests that assess for verb tense include the OWLS, CASL, CELF, PLS, and SPELT tests.

Elicitation Ideas

Continue reading →

Language Research – Making Research Relevant

A common gripe for a long time with research of language disorders is that much of it has often been irrelevant to the actual teaching of language.  And by often, I mean nearly always.  A lot of the research seems geared toward one isolated characteristic of one subset of one small segment of people, and only to that particular population.  The similarity of the following fake titles to actual titles may help demonstrate my point…

- “Toward Understanding Morphologic Tendencies in Left Handed Nicaraguan Preschoolers.”    or

- “Past Tense Comprehension in Bilingual Adopted Children; A Conceptual Framework.”

It just has long seemed that these are the sorts of research titles that usually exist in the most commonly read sources of language research.  And that’s when these journals even decide to address language at all, which has long seemed to be much less common then research addressing say, hearing, or voice, or stuttering.  And this is too bad.  Because there are many, many language related questions out there that can be addressed scientifically that would actually be useful in teaching language.  So what might these “practical” studies look like?  Here are some ideas I’ve had.

Question:  Do twins often have one member with more language deficits than the other?

Implication:  This often seems to be the case.  Anecdotally, it seems as though one twin often speaks for the other, almost creating deficits in the less talkative twin.  If the research would support this hypothesis, then we could prepare for this with extra early intervention, and assistance for twin parents.

Continue reading →

Why We Learn Language

What follows is an abbreviated list of some linguistic aspects of the English language along with their functions. As with any system of symbols, such as drawing, math, and language, this list is meant to convey an interpretation of reality. Just as the Mona Lisa is not the actual woman used as the model, squiggles on a map of England are not the actual England, and buildings and roads are composed of bricks and mortar rather than the equations used to combine building materials, this list is meant merely as a representation. Significant overlap exists. And as with any symbolic representation, any part of it that does not assist may reasonably be ignored. Keep in mind that the functions answer the question of why we learn the units. For example: Why do we learn idioms? To provide flexibility, creativity, and social status to our language.

Untitledddv

Idiom of the Month

What idiom is this not?

beating around bush 1

Targeting Specific Structures in Reading

Earlier, I was tutoring some speech kids working on reading. I just happened to have some comprehension flash cards targeting comprehension – oral or reading – of specific targets. These kids needed help with reading more than oral language, and so because I don’t have a lot of materials targeting reading, I decided to use the cards. One deck had about 12 cards with negative contractions (can’t, aren’t, isn’t, etc.), and the other deck had regular plurals.
Bottom line – this activity rocked. The kids missed the first couple. I told them to focus on “those tricky word endings,” (you know the kind that so many speech and language kids miss in oral language), and after struggling with the next few cards, by the end, they were getting it with no problems. They’d improved right then and there.
That got me thinking. These kids didn’t have deficits with plurals and contractions in oral language. But they did in reading. And I bet they did in writing too. They used to have these kind of errors in oral language, and we know from the research that young kids with speech and language deficits often turn into kids with reading deficits. I’ve never seen anybody targeting specific language structures like these in reading, but I’m pretty sure it would be a good idea.

Teaching Functions

functions pic for blog

Here’s one way to practice identifying functions.

I don’t think I’ve ever seen anyone specifically teach functions.  Well, maybe, but definitely it seems rare.  Which is a shame, because functions are huge in expressive language and semantics specifically.  If you want someone to be better at describing, one of the best things you can do is teach functions.  And if you want somebody to be better at expressive language, one of the best things you can do is to work on improving describing.  Just a little contemplation can reveal just how common functions are in describing.  Filling in the blank in “What’s a _______?” for so many things requires a function for the answer.

What’s a refrigerator?  It’s an appliance (category) that keeps food cold (function).

What’s a ruler?  It’s something that measures length (function).

What are quotation marks?  They’re a type of punctuation (category) that shows that somebody is saying something (function).

Come to think of it, maybe I need to do a blog post about how we should teach categories more too.  Anyway, assessing for deficits in using functions are common in tests and screens such as the DIAL, the CELF, and the PLS tests, so it’s easy to figure out if a kid has difficulties in this area.  Someday good language therapy will include teaching functions to kids we’ve identified as having function using deficits.  Hopefully that day won’t be too far away.

Why Are There So Few Male SLPs?

This question seems to perplex many, and the numbers do seem pretty staggering.  Recent figures are something like 96% of SLPs being women in the U.S., with similar numbers abroad.  As a male speech-language pathologist I thought I would chip in a few reasons why I believe these numbers are the way they are.

Some of these reasons have been offered before.  They include

  • There’s a perception of low opportunity and pay, especially considering the cost of the at least six years of college education required to become an SLP.  This has been extensively discussed, such as here and here.  Generally, there seems to be some justification for the perception, though it’s probably true that opportunity and pay is decreasing in many professions as the middle class continues its long decline.
  • Gender roles and expectations definitely play a part.  Fortunately, I had someone who knew about the field that suggested speech and language pathology as a possibility for me – a college professor trying to finally give me some direction as I was just about to graduate with a degree in communication.  I had never even considered this profession, nor did I even know about it, before I was 22 years old.  I’m guessing that more women than men have speech pathology suggested to them by others thanks at least in some part to gender expectations.
  • Speech pathology is a helping, nurturing profession, which tends to attract women.  Many men just tend to think that they can’t derive as much satisfaction from helping others.  I obviously disagree, but I do understand how it took me so long of my own life to realize this.

I have a few additional reasons contributing to the huge disparity which I haven’t seen before.

  • A big one is recognition.  I think men crave recognition more than women, and teaching, nursing, SLPs, and similar professions are just not appreciated nearly enough.  Individual greatness in these professions tends to go unacknowledged, as does the importance of the professions’ overall contributions to society.  Men want to be heroes.  This is genetic and evolutionarily driven.  Meanwhile, our society tends to idolize men in some professions – sports, military, etc. – while ignoring the hard work, sacrifice, and importance of people in helping professions.
  • Many men are timid about working with kids.  Many SLPs work with kids.  Every now and then the media decides to start focusing on (what I believe to be) the extremely small amount of male teachers who do something inappropriate with children, which causes suspicion of all male teachers that I believe is inaccurate relative to the actual risk.
  • There is also some genetic, inherent component to women enjoying being around kids more, and especially younger kids.  This really shouldn’t be ignored as a contributing factor, but it usually is.  My experience has been that many male SLPs end up working with older children, I think in some part because of inexperience dealing with the behaviors of younger kids.

So one question then always arises in these discussions.  What can be done to attract more males to speech pathology?  Some contributing causes are more easily addressed, while others may take a long time.  We can try to get the word out to more males.  We can try to better prepare males, especially in dealing with behavior issues in the population SLPs often work with.  I think though, unfortunately, that the most biggest hurdle to overcoming this issue may with the factor that contributes the most.  Societal expectations need some shifting.  We can start by taking cues from how many people react to those in certain other professions.  When we start seeing things like special parking spaces for teachers and words like “That’s great!” when hearing that someone is a nurse or SLP, then maybe we’ll start seeing more males in these professions.

 

What’s Wrong With Our Goals for Following Directions? (.)

Goals for following directions are commonly seen in Individualized Education Plans (IEPs) of language disordered kids.  Anecdotally speaking, they’re probably the most common language goal.   Most of them tend to have some serious problems though, which perhaps we can illuminate from a quick analysis of one or two examples.  And there’s one thing that’s really conspicuous by its absence.  We’ll see if we can use these examples to figure it out.

Example 1:  “Paul will follow two step directions in the classroom for three consecutive days.”   What’s wrong with this?  Plenty, actually, but one thing especially.  Let’s say that for three straight days Paul is handed some trash, and after receiving the directions, “Paul, take this to the trash can and throw it away,” he does it.  Goal achieved, right?  There’s two steps and three days in the classroom, after all.

Symbol of my example, or of these goals?

Symbolic picture

But what if Paul is twelve years old?  What if Paul has been throwing away trash like this for years?  Maybe he has done it so often, he knows what to do merely by giving him trash.  He may not have even been listening to the directions.  In this case, Paul has not been taught, nor has he accomplished anything that he wasn’t already able to do, despite having “achieved his goal.”   The goal did not include something measurable that he wasn’t already able to do.  And there still is that one thing that’s seriously missing.

Example 2:  Mary will follow multi-step directions in the classroom or wherever at some percentage.  Okay, this admittedly is a softball example, but it’s one I’ve seen plenty.  What’s wrong?  It’s not specific enough.  I’m assuming it means more than two, but it doesn’t really say.  Again too we run across the difficulty of the fact that no two directions tend to be equal in difficulty.  “Get the ball and give it to me!” and “Please walk to the board and write the answer below the date,” are both notably two step directions.  But they’re vastly different in difficulty.  Which leads us to the one biggie that’s missing in these goals, and almost all following directions goals that I’ve ever seen.  It’s the language itself.

Continue reading →

Idiom of the Week

What idiom is this not? (Click picture for answer)

Untitled2

Skepticism in Communication Disorders – A Tale of Two Techniques

Oral Motor Therapy and Facilitated Communication

…it is far easier to come up with an idea than it is to do the grunt work of scientifically verifying if that idea is a good one.

The stories of both non speech oral motor exercises and facilitated communication are both fascinating, and have sometimes had, at least in the case of facilitated communication, tragic results.  So, then, what are the similarities?  And what can we learn from them?

Facilitated communication (FC) is the technique of using a helper’s hand to guide a severely disabled person’s fingers or hands to type or point at a letter board.  It is a practical requirement that the disabled person must not be able to communicate in any other way.  The facilitator is trained in ways to help hold and support, but theoretically not guide, the patient’s hand to enable communication.  FC was first enthusiastically embraced by many media outlets, and by the general special education community in the 1980′s and 1990′s, before being thoroughly refuted by research, and denounced by every major association that matters,  including the American Psychological Association and the Association for Science in Autism Treatment.  It seemed also that in many cases, these facilitators’ extreme desire to help their students led them to trump up stories of parental sexual abuse – stories that they didn’t seem to realize that they were concocting.  FC seemed to generally fade from the public consciousness, but lately it has been revived.  Unbelievably, Syracuse University helped its reemergence under the re-branding of “supported typing” by promoting its founder, Douglas Biklen to Dean of its School of Education, and by renaming its Facilitated Communication Institute as the School of Communication and Inclusion.  (Maybe these people have been trying to jump on the autism explosion gravy train that earlier FC had just missed?)

Here is a fantastic summary of facilitated communication.  Here is the American Speech-Language Pathology Association’s (ASHA) denunciation of FC.

While oral motor therapy has been less – let’s just say – notorious than FC, it has certainly garnered its fair share of criticism and controversy.

Continue reading →

Idiom of the Week

What idiom is this not?  (Click picture for answer)eye on sister literal
#SLP2bchat

Join the conversation, speechies!

Wellcome Trust Blog

Life from a Wellcome Trust perspective

Bowen Speech Blog

A blog focused on topics in speech, language, swallowing, and related disorders!

The SpeechEasy

School-based speech and language therapy ideas

Mind Hacks

Neuroscience and psychology news and views.

The Speech Guy

Tech Savvy Speech Therapy

so to Speak

the path to speech path

Scientia Salon

a webzine about philosophy and science

Speech 2U

A lively little speech & language blog

The Language Fix

A blog about sharing information related to language and learning

School SLP

A blog about sharing information related to language and learning

madamemadden

Salut/Hallo/Hola! After enjoying teaching MFL and leading my department, I've started an exciting new venture this year, as Couse Tutor for the PGCE MFL at Leeds Trinity University. I'm interested in developments in Language teaching and sharing ideas.

Speech Adventures

Speech-Language Therapy, Technology, and Humor

The Speech Dudes

The Edgy Side of Speech Pathology; sharp, profane, but never dull

In Spontaneous Speech

Teaching students to speak clearly and effectively.

Follow

Get every new post delivered to your Inbox.

Join 132 other followers